November 5, 1996
Dr. Joseph LeDoux: Using Rats to Trace Routes of Fear
By SANDRA BLAKESLEE
The realm of emotion and feeling is a treacherous one
for science. Rage, lust, envy and shame churn in the human
psyche. Yearning, disappointment and fear mingle with conscious
thought, sway decisions and then recede like phantoms.
Writers, psychoanalysts and psychologists may try to sort
out the interplay of cognition and desire, of thought and
compulsion. But understanding the origin and architecture
of human emotions in the brain is another matter altogether.
What is the key to linking emotions to activity in the cells
of the brain?
Rats -- better yet, frightened rats -- are the key, says
Dr. Joseph LeDoux, a 46-year-old neuroscientist at New York
University who pioneered the study of emotions as biological
phenomena.
"You start with something you can study," LeDoux said
in a recent interview. Like most animals, rats exhibit fear,
an emotion that may help creatures escape from predators.
In experiments over the last 15 years, LeDoux has traced
fear inside the rat's brain -- from the first sounds of
danger detected by the outer ear to inner brain circuits
that cause the animal either to freeze or to run for its
life.
Using this strategy, LeDoux has given researchers the
first real glimpse into the neuroanatomy of an emotion.
And though the work has been done in rats, Ledoux says the
findings also apply to humans, providing insights into why
it is so difficult to control emotions with rational, conscious
thought.
One of the biggest surprises from LeDoux's work is that
there may be no such thing as the limbic system -- a brain
structure that has been supposed to underlie emotion and
motivation. All students are taught about the limbic system,
LeDoux said, "but in my opinion, it's no longer a valid
concept."
Anatomists like LeDoux "are funny people," said Dr. Paul
MacLean, the scientist who coined the term limbic system.
"They think only about fear and rage," said MacLean, a senior
researcher at the National Institute of Mental Health's
Neuroscience Center in Washington. "They forget love, which
more than anything else accounts for the development of
the human race." The limbic system is still a valid concept,
he said, adding that efforts to discard the idea were ill-founded.
But Dr. James McGaugh, a neurobiologist at the University
of California in Irvine, has high praise for LeDoux. "Joe
LeDoux is among a small number of people pushing the frontiers
of how the brain provides a basis for our emotional responses,"
he said. "Emotions are not a dominant theme in neuroscience.
He's been a modern pioneer."
Dr. Michael Gazzaniga, a neuroscientist at Dartmouth College
in Hanover, N.H., said LeDoux had the courage to tackle
a subject -- understanding the neuroanatomy of emotions
in the brain -- that others thought was impossible to do.
"Joe jumped in," Gazzaniga said, "and has unearthed some
basic truths about the brain."
LeDoux may have ventured into a field that others avoided
because he entered graduate school with virtually no training
in biology. "I wasn't corrupted by too much knowledge,"
he said. "I was naive enough to think that emotions could
be studied."
Raised in rural Louisiana, LeDoux said that he was not
supposed to be a scientist. "My mother said she would pay
for my college education if I studied business, so I did,"
he said. "I hated it. I didn't know what to do when I graduated
from Louisiana State University, so I did more business
school. I was interested in consumer issues, in Ralph Nader
kinds of things, and wondered if the principles of the psychologist
B.F. Skinner might be useful for understanding consumer
behavior. So I wrote him a letter and he actually wrote
back. He said it wouldn't be ethical."
LeDoux laughed. "That was it," he said. "I left business
school with no regrets."
Looking for something new, LeDoux volunteered to work
in a laboratory where he helped carry out experiments on
rats and signed up for a course on the neural basis of learning
and memory. He loved it, applied to 20 graduate schools
and, with the help of his adviser, was accepted at just
one, the State University of New York at Stony Brook.
Soon afterward, in 1974 and 1975, experiments carried
out on so-called split brains attracted his attention. In
people who had had the connection between the left and right
brain hemispheres severed, the left side often did not know
what the right side was doing. But emotional information
seemed to be leaking across the hemispheres, suggesting
a different sort of wiring than for language or movement.
"I thought why not study emotions?" LeDoux said.
He said that at the time cognitive scientists tended to
confuse emotions and feelings. LeDoux says that emotions
are hard-wired, biological functions of the nervous system
that evolved to help animals survive in hostile environments
and procreate. The emotional systems underlying fearful,
sexual or feeding behaviors are pretty similar across species,
though each emotion may have its own separate neural wiring.
Feelings, in LeDoux's scheme of things, are "red herrings,"
products of the conscious mind, labels we give to unconscious
emotions. "What we really want to study is the brain system
that generates emotions," LeDoux said, not the higher brain
systems that read meaning into them. Because many researchers
thought they first had to understand consciousness and feelings,
he said, they were intimidated and stayed away from emotions.
In his new book, "The Emotional Brain" published this
month by Simon & Schuster, LeDoux also describes how cognitive
neuroscientists have tended to separate cognition and emotion
as separate facets of mind. Since feelings are subjective,
he said, they are very difficult to study objectively.
But that is not the case with emotions, LeDoux said. One
can study how emotions are processed in the brain without
reference to more complicated feelings. Having identified
an emotion like fear, the questions are: What brain cells
detect danger? How do they do it? What is the wiring diagram?
What brain chemicals are used?
In 1977, LeDoux went to Cornell University Medical College
to begin his rat studies and has continued them at New York
University's Center for Neural Science since 1989. In the
experiments, rats are exposed to a tone and mild electric
shock at the same time. Later, at the sound of the tone
by itself, they freeze, as if frightened. They have been
conditioned to fear the noise.
While many researchers use this strategy to study memory
and learning in animals, LeDoux has focused all his attention
on emotions. "My approach was to let the natural flow of
information through the brain be my guide," he said.
"I started where the sound stimulus enters the brain and
tried to trace the pathways forward," not knowing in advance
where they went, he said.
By using tracers, chemicals that stain neurons and thus
show where they send their fibers, LeDoux found a direct
pathway from the ear to a way station called the sensory
thalamus that led directly to the amygdala, an almond-shaped
structure in the forebrain. When this pathway was cut, rats
could not be conditioned to fear a sound.
Most of the time, the amygdala is quiet, LeDoux said.
But when it receives a strong stimulus, it sets sirens in
motion. Hairs stand on end, the heart races and fight or
flight hormones flood the body.
On closer examination, LeDoux found that the amygdala
is designed to detect predators. For example, when rats
are threatened, they emit very high frequency (20,000 to
30,000 cycles per second) screams. When another rat hears
this scream, a signal goes from the auditory cortex, where
sounds are processed, directly to the amygdala. In other
words, says LeDoux, when these sound waves penetrate the
rat brain, the amygdala is instantly activated even though
it does not "know" the sound is coming from another rat.
The human brain is similarly wired, LeDoux said. A visual
stimulus, perhaps the sight of a snake on a dirt path, will
travel to the amygdala in a few thousandths of a second.
The human amygdala contains cells that fire in response
to expressions on faces and may also react to objects of
fear.
But, LeDoux said, the amygdala is specialized for reacting
to stimuli and triggering a physiological response, a process
that he would describe as the "emotion" of fear. That is
distinct from a conscious feeling of fear, LeDoux said.
Feelings, he said, arise from a second, slower pathway that
travels from the ear to the amygdala and then on to the
higher cortex. There, the frightening stimulus is analyzed
in detail, using information from many parts of the brain,
and a message is sent back down to the amygdala.
If the message is a false alarm -- hey, it is a stick
and not a snake -- the cortex will try to abort the amygdala's
alarm signals. But the person will have felt a jolt because
of the initial arousal of the amygdala.
This double pathway is very different from the limbic
system that is taught to every biology student, LeDoux said.
The limbic system is a hypothetical construct of pathways
in the forebrain, which contains the hippocampus, amygdala
and a few other tiny structures, that supposedly gets all
sorts of sensory input from the external world -- sight,
smell, hearing, touch and taste -- as well as from the viscera.
When these sensations are integrated in the limbic system,
emotional experiences occur.
The basic idea that there is a system for dealing with
emotion that is divorced from cognition is a good one, LeDoux
said. But when the fear circuit is actually traced, he said,
the hippocampus and other "limbic" structures do not play
much of a role. The amygdala alone sits at the center. Sensory
information comes in and motor commands are sent out.
"This is how the brain does it," LeDoux said. Emotion
and cognition are separate but interacting mental functions
mediated by separate but interacting brain systems.
Such double wiring can create problems for people, LeDoux
said. Neural connections from the cortex down to the amygdala
are less well developed than are connections from the amygdala
back up to the cortex. Thus, the amygdala exerts a greater
influence on the cortex than vice versa. Once an emotion
is turned on, it is difficult for the cortex to turn it
off.
"This is why we have trouble controlling our emotions,"
LeDoux said. "They can really trip us up."
Fear is just one emotion, LeDoux said. The amygdala has
12 to 15 distinct regions and only two so far have been
clearly implicated in fear. Other emotions might reside
in the similar circuits but their anatomy has not been traced.
Another emotion well-suited to the rat brain tracing technique
is sexual arousal. In humans, sexuality may be linked to
love, which LeDoux would classify as a feeling rather than
an emotion. While he is confident about the ability to understand
sexual attraction's neural basis in rats and humans, he
is not suggesting work on the neurobiology of love -- at
least, not until the deeper puzzle of what gives rise to
consciousness and feelings is solved.
Copyright 1996 The New York Times Company
Courtesy of The
New York Times Online
|